Telesynth

Study to determine how well older people can understand telecare medication reminders under adverse circumstances, such as high background noise.

Aims

  • To determine how well older people with and without hearing problems can understand medication reminders when they hear them under adverse circumstances (e.g., high background noise, bad phoneline).
  • To get older peoples' views on how to design telecare reminders. 

We focused on medications because medication names are extremely tricky words to understand and remember. They are infrequent, latinate, and long.

Yet, replacing the names with descriptions such as “the little blue pill” is not safe. Many people with chronic conditions are on generic medications that are produced by several different companies – packaging and pills look different depending on the manufacturer to the other.

In this study, we worked with older people, because this age group is more likely to be on medication, and may have quite complex medication regimes. Older people also have a much broader range of sensory and cognitive ability than younger people – therefore, testing a solution with older people is a good way of making it accessible to a large proportion of the population.

Image
Older adults sat with small dog

Method

How was the study done?

We tested how well people could remember medication reminders that were generated by one human and two computer voices.

Reminders were either for one or four medications at a time. In some of the four-medication reminders, the medication names were repeated, in some, an explanation was added. In the reminders, we used both over-the-counter and prescription medications, and we checked beforehand to what extent participants were familiar with the medications.

Some examples of reminders: 

  • “Please remember to take the following medication: Aspirin.” 
  • “Please remember to take the following four medications: Paracetamol, Aspirin, Corsodyl, and Metformin.” 
  • “Please remember to take the following four medications: Paracetamol, Aspirin, Corsodyl, and Metformin. I repeat: Paracetamol, Aspirin, Corsodyl, and Metformin.” 
  • “Please remember to take the following four medications: Paracetamol, for your pain, Aspirin, to thin your blood, Corsodyl, for your mouth ulcer, and Metformin, for your diabetes.”

Participants were recruited from four general practices in Edinburgh.

First, they rated how natural the voices sounded and provided comments in a short interview. Then, they heard 72 reminders. Each reminder was followed by a short sentence, such as “Lucy sees four blue ships.” After they had repeated the sentence, they were asked to pick out the medications they’d heard from a list of 24 medication names, complete with potential indications. Only 12 of those occurred in the reminders, the remaining 12 were distractors. 

Participants heard reminders in one of four conditions:

  • high background noise / bad simulated telephone line
  • high background noise / good simulated telephone line
  • low background noise / bad simulated telephone line
  • low background noise / low simulated telephone line

Before the voice tests, we assessed participants’ hearing and asked them to do several memory, attention, and recognition tasks.

56 participants completed the study, and 44 of these fulfilled the inclusion criteria (no hearing aids, no hearing loss due to middle ear hearing loss, no severe hearing loss). 

Results

What did we find?

Overall, people could remember one medication fairly well, but only if they already knew it. Medications were more difficult to remember when there was a lot of background noise, but there was no difference between the voices. 

When hearing four medications, people mostly only remembered the one or two they knew. The human voice had a slight advantage over the computer-generated voices. Medications were remembered a little better when they were explained, and best when the names were repeated.

When we spoke to our participants after they’d completed the task, the feedback was very similar to what we found in the 2011 study. Some liked the idea of getting reminders over the phone, some preferred getting texts, while others liked to tick things off on paper.

It was important for people to be able to review the information they’d gotten and ask for further information if they needed it. People who took many medications managed because they wove them tightly into their daily routine. 

What does this mean in practice?

It is feasible to give people spoken reminders over the telephone even if they have mild hearing problems, but these reminders need to be about something they already know, and they should be reinforced by other measures such as compartment boxes.

It also helps to talk to people about their routine – if this is more or less regular, finding a way to integrate the medications with it might be all they need.

Finally, since people prefer to receive reminders in different ways, reminder systems should use different channels, e.g. texting and phoning.

Paper

Can older people remember medication reminders presented using synthetic speech? (Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 2015)

 

Funder Chief Scientist Office
Chief Investigator Professor Brian McKinstry